An overview of pain
The International Association
for the Study of Pain defines pain as ‘an unpleasant sensory and emotional
experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in
terms of such damage’. Pain is a potent negative affective state that focuses an
animal’s attention and biases its behavior.
One of the problems with
assessing pain in animals is that pain can only be measured indirectly; while
humans can self-assess their levels of pain and verbally report pain scores,
the subjective experiences of animals are particularly difficult to assess. An
animal in pain will withdraw from the source of the insult if it can be
identified, protect the area affected both through immobilization and active defensive
aggression and may communicate the pain to others through changes in facial
expression, body postures and vocalizations.
By contrast, health and
happiness are identifiable by an open and relaxed posture, facial expressions of
contentment and production of chemicals that are associated with pleasure, such
as endorphins. The ability to recognize and to respond to painful stimuli has
evolved to protect individuals against tissue damage and provides information
to safeguard against dangerous or threatening stimuli in the future. Pain may
be associated with suffering at many different levels, depending on both the
circumstance and the cognitive ability of the animal concerned. At its simplest
it may be a temporary negative state, which guides the animal’s withdrawal from
a noxious stimulus.
A variety of animals may be
able to anticipate pain and generate feelings of anxiety when faced with a
predictably painful stimulus and will take avoidance action as appropriate.
This will cause the activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary axis and behaviorally
might include threatening behavior or attempts at escape (fight or flight
response). The intensity of the response is usually directly related to the
intensity of the perceived threat. It is important to realize that the perceived
threat arises from a combination of factors (e.g. previous experience,
sensitivity to pain, emotional state) without a single cause; and as a result of
the accumulation of several risk factors within the three levels of behavior assessment
discussed in the previous section. Therefore, simply approaching the animal may
not seem threatening from the person’s perspective, but very threatening from the
animal’s perspective. It is also thought by some that certain species such as
horses and dogs may be capable of a pain phobia; this involves the generation
of an ungraded and extreme reaction in response to even the most low grade sign
of any pain.
While pain phobias may
exist, they should be distinguished from extreme responses that have been
conditioned and allodynia. This is an exaggerated pain response to normally innocuous
stimuli, and although mechanisms are unknown, allodynia probably arises in the structures
of the limbic system of the brain, such as the amygdala and periaqueductal
grey, which are associated with the processing of emotions.
Animals showing an extreme response
for whatever reason are potentially very dangerous and require specialist
intervention in consultation with a veterinary behaviorist. An even higher
cognitive level of response to pain is pain empathy, i.e. responding to the pain
of others and many owners may report that their pets are capable of this,
although it remains to be demonstrated scientifically.
Pain is also often classified
according to its temporal pattern and this is associated with different
psychological impacts and behavioural tendencies, which might be apparent in a
range of species. In humans, individual painful episodes may be referred to as
per-acute pain episodes and are behaviorally characterized by vocalization and
withdrawal of the painful area. Acute pain refers to episodes that last up to
about 3 weeks and are associated with fear and anxiety, reduced activity and
care-soliciting behaviour. Subacute pain lasts for between 3 and 12 weeks and
is characterized by oscillating bouts of activity and inactivity, signs of
frustration (including irritability) and the development of coping strategies
associated with longer term adaptation to the pain. Early signs of depression
may also become apparent at this time.
Beyond this, the pain may be
considered chronic and depression, together with other passive coping
strategies, is more likely. Often subacute episodes may occur against a
background of chronic pain in individuals with longstanding musculoskeletal
lesions, and in the horse this may present as periodic bucking set against a ‘loss
of spirit’. While the changes over time may partly reflect natural adaptive
developmental changes to an unresolved lesion, it is important to recognise
that learning will also occur as a consequence of the responses made over time and
affect the response that is shown.
Reference: Animal Physiotherapy: Assessment, Treatment and
Rehabilitation of Animals
Edited by Catherine M. McGowan, Lesley
Goff, Narelle Stubbs
No comments:
Post a Comment