Assessing pain in animals
Pain
assessment involves the integration of measurements of behaviour and physiology
together with knowledge of the bi-directional mechanisms that control pain.
Morton and Griffiths (1985) proposed a framework for the recognition of pain,
distress and discomfort based on a combined assessment of appearance, food and
water intake, behaviour, cardiovascular functioning, digestive system activity and
neurological/musculoskeletal signs. This provides a useful framework, but, the
correlation between physiological measures such as heart rate, respiratory rate
and pupil dilation versus subjective pain scores may be poor and there is a
need for greater validation of pain scales.
Given the enormous
range of individual factors that can affect pain perception in a given context
discussed above, it should be apparent that it is difficult to accurately
assess the pain of an individual without a thorough history, including baseline
assessments of behaviour and temperament. In addition, given the differences
that inevitably exist between assessors, it is also important that assessment
is repeated by the same assessor on all possible occasions, in order to reduce
this possible source of error.
Laboratory methods to
assess pain in domesticated animals might be thought of as being more objective
and are increasingly sophisticated. Clinical assessment generally relies on
evaluating a range of behavioural signs of pain, and these may be integrated
into subjective scoring systems. Verbal rating scales involve qualitative
description of behaviour observed, and simple quantitative scales involve
subjectively rating pain as No Pain,
Mild, Moderate or Severe. These assessment protocols have been
criticised not only for the large variation between different observers, but
also for their lack of sensitivity.
Numeric scales rating
pain between 0 and 10, and visual analogue scales marking pain on a ruler on
which 0 = No Pain Present and 100 = Worst Pain Imaginable, are generally
considered to provide better sensitivity and reliability. However, the validity
of these systems may be questioned owing to a lack of transparency regarding
pain parameters considered by observers, and these are weighted in the final
score. Some observers may reliably weight vocalisations heavily because of ease
of measurement and anthropomorphism, but these vocalisations may not correlate
well with pain experiences since dogs occasionally vocalise while under anesthesia
when pain is presumably prevented.
Although the science
of valid pain assessment in animals is in its infancy, this does not negate the
responsibility of those that work with animals in pain to institute and apply pain
assessment criteria within their practice. Given current knowledge, the
physiotherapist should at the very least use some form of pain scale that both
the owner and the physiotherapist can complete and keep a behavioural diary of
therapy sessions to monitor pain responses. Should there be any doubt that a
certain condition is painful, it is good practice to assume that what would be
painful for a person is painful for that animal (IRAC 1985). Further
information on the recognition and assessment of animal pain is hosted by the
University of Edinburgh at: www.vet.ed.ac.uk/animalpain/, and readers may wish
to refer to this for further detail of some of the principles that have been
discussed in this chapter.
Source of pain
|
Behavioural
response
|
General
responses
|
Lethargy
Reduction in
grooming
Depression
Reduced feeding,
drinking
Protection of
painful site
Vocalization (dog:
whining, growling; equine:
groaning)
Aggression
Hanging tail
Ear position
(equine: pinned ears)
Facial expression
(canine: furrowed brow;
equine: clenched
jaw, wrinkled muzzle)
Restlessness/weight
shifting between all limbs
|
Limb
|
Avoidance or
reduction in weight bearing
Abnormal gait
Head bobbing during
locomotion
Rubbing, licking
wound site
Weight
shifting away from painful limb
|
Abdominal/ Spinal/ Visceral
pain
|
Tucked up posture
Glancing or nosing
abdomen
Abnormal stance,
stretching of hind limbs
Restlessness
Sweating
Trembling
|
Head pain
|
Headshaking and
facial rubbing
Head shyness
Grimacing
Signs often
exacerbated by exercise
Intranasal pain
Snorting and
sneezing
Turning of the
upper lip
Intra-oral pain
Reduced appetite
and/or dropping of food
being chewed
Teeth grinding
|
Reference: Animal Physiotherapy:
Assessment, Treatment and Rehabilitation of Animals
Edited by Catherine M. McGowan, Lesley Goff, Narelle Stubbs.
we should always know about the pain of the animal while treating them....more informations are given here..great..
ReplyDelete